Critical histories can be applied to policies, practices, or institutions (among other phenomena). They can be constructed from archival and textual data, and/or from microhistories (a focus on a single person or place). Critical histories require the researcher to apply a critical theoretical framework (such as CRT, feminism, critical democracy, etc.) to analyze the data collected.
Examples:
Gilroy, P. & McNamara, O. (2009). A critical history of research assessment in the UK and its post-1992 impact on education. Journal of Education for Teaching: International Research and Pedagogy, 35(4), 321-335.
Reid, D.K. & Knight, M.G. (2006). Disability justifies exclusion of minority students: A critical history grounded in disability studies. Educational Researcher, 35(6), 18-23.
Saturday, June 22, 2013
Critical History
Friday, June 14, 2013
Levels of Evidence
It's useful to consider the varying levels of evidence when thinking about the strength of research. The levels in the figure below come from the medical literature - and tend to privilege a positivist research perspective. Notice that the methodological design of the research plays a role in its level - and more generalizable, quantitative results are privileged. This is one perspective, and it is contested, especially in the social sciences!
From Pinto, L., Spares, S. & Driscoll, L. (2012). 95 Strategies for Remodeling Instruction. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
From Pinto, L., Spares, S. & Driscoll, L. (2012). 95 Strategies for Remodeling Instruction. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Saturation
Conclusions in qualitative research are drawn from patterns
a researcher identifies in the data, or conclusions can uncover
conceptual (NOT statistical) relationships. As such, we
look for points of saturation to know when we have collected and analyzed enough data – and
this can determine the sample size and the point at which analysis should end.
For more information, see the links above, and
Data saturation is the point where new data and theirsorting only confirm the categories (often numbering between three and six orso), themes, and conclusions already reached. Onwuegbuzie and Leech (2007) also
discuss theoretical saturation (Strauss & Corbin, 1990), and informational
redundancy (Lincoln & Guba, 1985) as specific areas of saturation. There are various strategies for determining when saturation is reached, but researchers should consider a codebook to track themes and findings.
For more information, see the links above, and
Onwuegbuzie, A.,
& Leech, N. L. (2007). A Call for Qualitative Power Analyses. Quality
& Quantity, 41,105–121. DOI 10.1007/s11135-005-1098-1
Some Sampling Methods Summarized
This is a general overview - please refer to other sources for details. The summary here is based on:
Onwuegbuzie, A.,
& Leech, N. L. (2007). A Call for Qualitative Power Analyses. Quality
& Quantity, 41,105–121. DOI 10.1007/s11135-005-1098-1
Random Sampling
Sampling type
|
Description
|
Notes
|
Simple Random Sampling
|
Participants selected so every person in the population
has the same chance of selection, and selection is independent (the selection
of 1 doesn’t affect the selection of others). Random means that each person
in the population is assigned a number, and the selection is based on a
random table of numbers. This is different from the “conversational” use of
the term random.
|
Requires an accurate list of the entire population – so if
the population was “science teachers in the GTA,” the researcher would have
to have a list of ALL science teachers including contact information, and
draw from that
|
Stratified Random Sampling
|
Similar to above, but population is divided into homogenous
sub-populations (e.g., males and females) and the sub-populations are
randomized and selected
|
See above
|
Cluster Random Sampling
|
As above, but groups or clusters are randomly selected
|
As above
|
Systematic Random Sampling
|
As above, but the researcher selects every kth sampling
frame member,
where k represents the population size divided by the
desired sample size
|
As above
|
Multi-stage Random Sampling
|
The researcher samples in two or more stages because
either the population is relatively large or its members cannot easily be
identified
|
|
Non-Random Sampling
Sampling type
|
Description
|
Notes
|
Purposive Maximum Variation Sampling
|
A wide range of individuals, groups, or settings is
purposively selected so that different and divergent perspectives are
represented
|
The researcher has to have expert knowledge of and access
to the population
|
Purposive Homogeneous Sampling
|
sampling individuals, groups, or settings because they all
possess
similar characteristics or attributes
|
|
Purposive Critical Case Sampling
|
individuals, groups, or settings are selected that
bring to the fore the phenomenon of interest such that the
researcher can
learn more about the phenomenon than would have been
learned without including these critical cases
|
As above
|
Theory-based sampling
|
individuals, groups, or settings are selected because they
help the researcher to develop or expand a theory
|
|
Confirming and Disconfirming Case Samples
|
This is often applied at the end of data collection based
on what the individual cases said
|
Occurs at the end of the research process, in combination
with another sampling method
|
Snowball Sampling
|
Asking participants who have already been selected for the
study to recruit
other participants.
|
Occurs during data collection
|
Extreme Case Samples
|
an outlying case or one with
more extreme characteristics is studied
|
|
Intensity Sampling
|
researcher studies individuals, groups, or settings that
experience the phenomenon intensely but not extremely
|
|
Typical Case Sampling
|
researcher should consult several experts in the field of
study in order to obtain a consensus as to what example(s) is typical of the
phenomenon
|
Requires access to “experts” for consensus
|
Politically Important Sampling
|
researcher selects informants to be included/excluded
because they connect with politically sensitive issue
|
|
Random Purposeful Sampling
|
the researcher chooses cases at random (see above for
clarification on the formal definition of “random”) from the sampling frame
consisting of a purposefully selected sample. The researcher first obtains a
list of individuals of interest for study using one of the 15 other methods of
purposeful sampling, and then randomly selects cases
|
|
Stratified Purposeful Sampling
|
As above, but the selection is stratified (see above for
the definition of stratified)
|
|
Criterion Sampling
|
individuals, groups, or settings are selected that meet
criteria central to the research
|
|
Opportunistic Sampling
|
the researcher capitalizes on opportunities during data
collection stage to select cases. Cases could represent typical, negative,
critical, or extreme cases
|
|
Mixed Purposeful Sampling
|
mixing of more than one sampling
strategy (e.g., one extreme case sample and another
critical case sample). Results can be compared to
triangulate data
|
|
Convenience Sampling
|
selecting individuals or groups that happen to be
available and are willing to participate at the time
|
|
Quota Sampling
|
Cases selected based on specific characteristics and
quotas
|
A main limitation is that only those accessible at the
time of selection have a chance of being selected
|
How many? Sampling in qualitative research
Onwuegbuzie and Leech (2007) stress that though qualitative research
typically relies on small samples, the sample size is important because it
determines the extent to which the researcher can make generalizations. Sample
sizes in qualitative research should small enough so that the researcher can
extract thick, rich data, but also large enough that saturation (data,
theoretical saturation, and informational redundancy) are achieved (Lincoln
& Guba, 1985; Onwuegbuzie & Leech, 2007).
Mason (2010) cites Guest, Bunce And Johnson ‘s (2006)
finding that only 7 sources provided guidelines for qualitative sample sizes.
They are:
Source
|
Methodology
|
Sample Size
|
Morse, J.M. (1994). Designing funded qualitative research.
In Norman K. Denzin & Yvonna S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative research (2nd ed., pp.220-35).
|
Ethnography and ethnoscience
|
30-50 interviews
|
Bernard, H.R. (2000). Social research methods.
|
Ethnography and ethnoscience
|
30-60 interviews
|
Creswell, J. (1998). Qualitative inquiry and research
design: Choosing among five traditions.
|
Grounded theory
|
20-30 interviews
|
Morse (1994)
|
Grounded theory
|
30-50 interviews
|
Cresswell (1998)
|
Phenomenology
|
5-25 interviews
|
Morse (1994)
|
Phenomenology
|
At least 6 interviews
|
Bertaux, D. (1981). From the life-history approach to
the transformation of sociological practice. In D. Bertaux (Ed.), Biography and society: The life history
approach in the social sciences (pp.29-45).
|
All qualitative
|
At lease 15
|
Sources cited:
Guest, G., Bunce, A., & Johnson, L. (2006). "Howmany interviews are enough? An experiment with data saturation andvariability". Field Methods, 18(1), 59-82.
Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic
inquiry. Beverly Hills , CA : Sage.
Mason, M. (2010). Sample Size and Saturation in PhD Studies
Using Qualitative Interviews. Forum
Qualitative Sozialforschung / Forum: Qualitative Social Research, 11(3), Art.
8, http://nbnresolving.de/urn:nbn:de:0114-fqs100387
Onwuegbuzie, A.,
& Leech, N. L. (2007). Sampling designs in qualitative research:
Making the sampling process more public. The Qualitative Report, 12(2),
238-254. Retrieved [Insert date], from
http://www.nova.edu/ssss/QR/QR12-2/onwuegbuzie1.pdf
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)